首页 > 代码库 > What does a Bayes factor feel like?(转)

What does a Bayes factor feel like?(转)

A Bayes factor (BF) is a statistical index that quantifies the evidence for a hypothesis, compared to an alternative hypothesis (for introductions to Bayes factors, see here, here or here).

Although the BF is a continuous measure of evidence, humans love verbal labels, categories, and benchmarks. Labels give interpretations of the objective index – and that is both the good and the bad about labels. The good thing is that these labels can facilitate communication (but see @richardmorey), and people just crave for verbal interpretations to guide their understanding of those “boring” raw numbers.

技术分享

The bad thing about labels is that an interpretation should always be context dependent (Such as “30 min.” can be both a long time (train delay) or a short time (concert), as @CaAl said). But once a categorical system has been established, it’s no longer context dependent.

 

These labels can also be a dangerous tool, as they implicitly introduce cutoff values (“Hey, the BF jumped over the boundary of 3. It’s not anecdotal any more, it’s moderate evidence!”). But we do not want another sacred .05 criterion!; see also Andrew Gelman’s blog post and its critical comments. The strength of the BF is precisely its non-binary nature.

Several labels for paraphrasing the size of a BF have been suggested. The most common system seems to be the suggestion of Harold Jeffreys (1961):

Bayes factor 技术分享Label
> 100Extreme evidence for H1
30 – 100Very strong evidence for H1
10 – 30Strong evidence for H1
3 – 10Moderate evidence for H1
1 – 3Anecdotal evidence for H1
1No evidence
1/3 – 1Anecdotal evidence for H0
1/3 – 1/10Moderate evidence for H0
1/10 – 1/30Strong evidence for H0
1/30 – 1/100Very strong evidence for H0
< 1/100Extreme evidence for H0

 

Note: The original label for 3 < BF < 10 was “substantial evidence”. Lee and Wagenmakers (2013) changed it to “moderate”, as “substantial” already sounds too decisive. “Anecdotal” formerly was known as “Barely worth mentioning”.

Kass and Raftery suggested a comparable classification, only that the “strong evidence” category for them starts at BF > 20 (see also Wikipedia entry).

Getting a feeling for Bayes factors

How much is a 技术分享 of 3.7? It indicates that data occured 3.7x more likely under 技术分享 than under 技术分享, given the priors assumed in the model. Is that a lot of evidence for 技术分享? Or not?

Following Table 1, it can be labeled “moderate evidence” for an effect – whatever that means.

Some have argued that strong evidence, such as BFs > 10, are quite evident from eyeballing only:

“If your result needs a statistician then you should design a better experiment.” (attributed to Ernest Rutherford)

 

Is that really the case? Can we just “see” it when there is an effect?

Let’s approach the topic a bit more experientially. What does such a BF look like, visually? We take the good old urn model as a first example.

Visualizing Bayes factors for proportions

Imagine the following scenario: When I give a present to my two boys (4 and 6 years old), it is not so important what it is. The most important thing is: “Is it fair?”. (And my boys are very sensitive detectors of unfairness).

Imagine you have bags with red and blue marbles. Obviously, the blue marbles are much better, so it is key to make sure that in each bag there is an equal number of red and blue marbles. Hence, for our familial harmony I should check whether reds and blues are distributed evenly or not. In statistical terms: 技术分享p = 0.5, 技术分享p != 0.5.

When drawing samples from the bags, the strongest evidence for an even distribution (技术分享) is given when exactly the same number of red and blue marbles has been drawn. How much evidence for 技术分享 is it when I draw n=2, 1 red/1 blue? The answer is in Figure 1, upper table, first row: The 技术分享 is 0.86 in favor of 技术分享, resp. a技术分享 of 1.16 in favor of 技术分享 – i.e., anecdotal evidence for an equal distribution.

You can get these values easily with the famous BayesFactor package for R:

proportionBF(y=1, N=2, p=0.5)

 

What if I had drawn two reds instead? Then the BF would be 1.14 in favor of 技术分享 (see Figure 1, lower table, row 1).

proportionBF(y=2, N=2, p=0.5)

Obviously, with small sample sizes it’s not possible to generate strong evidence, neither for 技术分享 nor for 技术分享. You need a minimal sample size to leave the region of “anecdotal evidence”. Figure 1 shows some examples how the BF gets more extreme with increasing sample size.

技术分享

Figure 1.

 

These visualizations indeed seem to indicate that for simple designs such as the urn model you do not really need a statistical test if your BF is > 10. You can just see it from looking at the data (although the “obviousness” is more pronounced for large BFs in small sample sizes).

Maximal and minimal Bayes factors for a certain sample size

The dotted lines in Figure 2 show the maximal and the minimal BF that can be obtained for a given number of drawn marbles. The minimum BF is obtained when the sample is maximally consistent with 技术分享 (i.e. when exactly the same number of red and blue marbles has been drawn), the maximal BF is obtained when only marbles from one color are drawn.

技术分享

Figure 2: Maximal and minimal BF for a certain sample size.

 

Figure 2 highlights two features:

  • If you have few data points, you cannot have strong evidence, neither for 技术分享 nor for 技术分享.
  • It is much easier to get strong evidence for 技术分享 than for 技术分享. This property depends somewhat on the choice of the prior distribution of 技术分享 effect sizes. If you expect very strong effects under the 技术分享, it is easier to get evidence for 技术分享. But still, with every reasonable prior distribution, it is easier to gather evidence for 技术分享.

 

Get a feeling yourself!

Here’s a shiny widget that let’s you draw marbles from the urn. Monitor how the BF evolves as you sequentially add marbles to your sample!

 

技术分享

[Open app in separate window]

Teaching sequential sampling and Bayes factors

技术分享

When I teach sequential sampling and Bayes factors, I bring an actual bag with marbles (or candies of two colors).

In my typical setup I ask some volunteers to test whether the same amount of both colors is in the bag. (The bag of course has a cover so that they don’t see the marbles). They may sample as many marbles as they want, but each marble costs them 10 Cent (i.e., an efficiency criterium: Sample as much as necessary, but not too much!). They should think aloud, about when they have a first hunch, and when they are relatively sure about the presence or absence of an effect. I use a color mixture of 2:1 – in my experience this give a good chance to detect the difference, but it’s not too obvious (some teams stop sampling and conclude “no difference”).

This exercise typically reveals following insights (hopefully!)

  • By intuition, humans sample sequentially. When the evidence is not strong enough, more data is sampled, until they are sure enough about the (un)fairness of the distribution.
  • Intuitionally, nobody does a fixed-n design with a-priori power analysis.
  • Often, they stop quite soon, in the range of “anecdotal evidence”. It’s also my own impression: BFs that are still in the “anecdotal” range already look quite conclusive for everyday hypothesis testing (e.g., a 2 vs. 9 distribution; 技术分享 = 2.7). This might change, however, if in the scenario a wrong decision is associated with higher costs. Next time, I will try a scenario of prescription drugs which have potentially severe side effects.

 

The “interocular traumatic test”

The analysis so far seems to support the “interocular traumatic test”: “when the data are so compelling that conclusion hits you straight between the eyes” (attributed to Joseph Berkson; quoted from Wagenmakers, Verhagen, & Ly, 2014).

But the authors go on and quote Edwards et al. (1963, p. 217), who said: “…the enthusiast’s interocular trauma may be the skeptic’s random error. A little arithmetic to verify the extent of the trauma can yield great peace of mind for little cost.”.

In the next visualization we will see, that large Bayes factors are not always obvious.

Visualizing Bayes factors for group differences

What happens if we switch to group differences? European women have on average a self-reported height of 165.8 cm, European males of 177.9 cm – difference: 12.1 cm, pooled standard deviation is around 7 cm. (Source:European Community Household Panel; see Garcia, J., & Quintana-Domeque, C., 2007; based on ~50,000 participants born between 1970 and 1980). This translates to a Cohen’s d of 1.72.

Unfortunately, this source only contains self-reported heights, which can be subject to biases (males over-report their height on average). But it was the only source I found which also contains the standard deviations within sex. However, Meyer et al (2001) report a similar effect size of d = 1.8 for objectively measured heights.

 

Now look at this plot. Would you say the blue lines are obviously higher than the red ones?

技术分享

I couldn’t say for sure. But the 技术分享 is 14.54, a “strong” evidence!

If we sort the lines by height the effect is more visible:

技术分享

… and alternatively, we can plot the distributions of males’ and females’ heights:技术分享

 

 

Again, you can play around with the interactive app:

技术分享

[Open app in separate window]

 

Can we get a feeling for Bayes factors?

To summarize: Whether a strong evidence “hits you between the eyes” depends on many things – the kind of test, the kind of visualization, the sample size. Sometimes a BF of 2.5 seems obvious, and sometimes it is hard to spot a BF>100 by eyeballing only. Overall, I’m glad that we have a numeric measure of strength of evidence and do not have to rely on eyeballing only.

Try it yourself – draw some marbles in the interactive app, or change the height difference between males and females, and calibrate your personal gut feeling with the resulting Bayes factor!

转自:http://www.nicebread.de/what-does-a-bayes-factor-feel-like/

 

What does a Bayes factor feel like?(转)